• Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar

Tracking Health and Wellness Applications of Brain Science

Spanish
sb-logo-with-brain
  • Resources
    • Monthly eNewsletter
    • Solving the Brain Fitness Puzzle
    • The SharpBrains Guide to Brain Fitness
    • How to evaluate brain training claims
    • Resources at a Glance
  • Brain Teasers
    • Top 25 Brain Teasers & Games for Teens and Adults
    • Brain Teasers for each Cognitive Ability
    • More Mind Teasers & Games for Adults of any Age
  • Virtual Summits
    • 2019 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
    • Speaker Roster
    • Brainnovations Pitch Contest
    • 2017 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
    • 2016 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
    • 2015 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
    • 2014 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
  • Report: Pervasive Neurotechnology
  • Report: Digital Brain Health
  • About
    • Mission & Team
    • Endorsements
    • Public Speaking
    • In the News
    • Contact Us

New Review of Neurofeedback Treatment for ADHD — Current State of the Science

January 17, 2012 by Dr. David Rabiner

neurofeedback adhd reviewsNeu­ro­feed­back — also known as EEG Biofeed­back — is an approach for treat­ing ADHD in which indi­vid­u­als are pro­vid­ed real-time feed­back on their brain­wave activ­i­ty and taught to alter their typ­i­cal EEG pat­tern to one that is con­sis­tent with a focused and atten­tive state. Accord­ing to neu­ro­feed­back pro­po­nents, this often results in improved atten­tion and reduced hyperactive/impulsive behavior.

Sev­er­al years ago I sum­ma­rized the sci­en­tif­ic sup­port for neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment — see here — and not­ed that although pos­i­tive find­ings had been report­ed in mul­ti­ple pub­lished stud­ies, lim­i­ta­tions of these stud­ies led many researchers to regard neu­ro­feed­back as a promis­ing, but unproven treatment.

The Amer­i­can Psy­cho­log­i­cal Asso­ci­a­tion has estab­lished a 5‑level sys­tem for grad­ing the evi­dence in sup­port of men­tal health treat­ments. These grades, and their asso­ci­at­ed lev­els of research sup­port, are as follows:

Lev­el 1 Not Empir­i­cal­ly Supported 

Sup­port­ed only through anec­do­tal evi­dence or non-peer reviewed case-studies.

Lev­el 2 Pos­si­bly Efficacious 

Shown to have a sig­nif­i­cant impact in at least one study, but the study lacked a ran­dom­ized assign­ment between controls.

Lev­el 3 Prob­a­bly Efficacious 

Shown to pro­duce pos­i­tive effects in more than one clin­i­cal, obser­va­tion­al wait list or with­in-sub­ject or between-sub­ject study.

Lev­el 4 Efficacious 

Shown to be more effec­tive than a no-treat­ment or place­bo con­trol group; the study must con­tain valid and clear­ly spec­i­fied out­come mea­sures, and it must be replic­a­ble by at least two inde­pen­dent researchers demon­strat­ing the same degree of efficacy.

Lev­el 5 Effi­ca­cious and Specific 

Shown to be sta­tis­ti­cal­ly supe­ri­or to cred­i­ble place­bo ther­a­pies or to actu­al treat­ments, and it must be shown as such in two or more inde­pen­dent studies.

Using the grad­ing sys­tem above, and based on stud­ies pub­lished through 2005, the con­clu­sion reached by the Pro­fes­sion­al Advi­so­ry Board of CHADD was that evi­dence sup­port­ing neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment for ADHD war­rant­ed a Lev­el 2, or ‘Pos­si­bly Effi­ca­cious’. You can read CHAD­D’s sum­ma­ry state­ment at www.help4adhd.org/en/treatment/complementary/WWK6A and I believe you will find this to be of interest.

Based on a research base that includes more recent­ly pub­lished stud­ies, how­ev­er, the con­clu­sions reached by Arn et. al., (2009) were far more pos­i­tive. These researchers con­duct­ed a meta-analy­sis of 15 stud­ies, 4 of which were report­ed to be ran­dom­ized con­trolled tri­als. Their con­clu­sion was that “Neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment for ADHD can be con­sid­ered ‘Effi­ca­cious and Spe­cif­ic’ (Lev­el 5) with a large effect size for inat­ten­tion and impul­siv­i­ty and a medi­um effect size for hyper­ac­tiv­i­ty.” This is a very dif­fer­ent con­clu­sion from the CHADD review and it is under­stand­able that par­ents, edu­ca­tors, and pro­fes­sion­als would be con­fused about the strength of the evi­dence base for neu­ro­feed­back treatment.

New Review Pro­vides Some Clarification

The Jour­nal of Atten­tion Dis­or­ders recent­ly pub­lished an updat­ed review of neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment for ADHD that helps clar­i­fy its sci­en­tif­ic sup­port [Loft­house et. al., (2011). A review of neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment for ADHD. Jour­nal of Atten­tion Dis­or­ders, pub­lished online 16 Novem­ber 2011. DOI: 10.1177/1087054711427530]. The authors include sci­en­tists who have con­duct­ed research tri­als of neu­ro­feed­back and also been part of the Mul­ti­modal Treat­ment Study of ADHD (MTA Study), the largest ADHD treat­ment study ever con­duct­ed. They are thus well equipped, in my view, to pro­vide a thor­ough and objec­tive review of a com­pli­cat­ed area.

The research base for their review was 14 stud­ies of neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment for chil­dren with ADHD in which par­tic­i­pants were ran­dom­ized to neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment or a con­trol con­di­tion. Eleven of these stud­ies were con­duct­ed between 2005 and 2010; this speaks to the strong accel­er­a­tion of neu­ro­feed­back research, which is a wel­come development.

Their review was lim­it­ed to those that ran­dom­ly assigned chil­dren to treat­ment or con­trol con­di­tions which is an essen­tial ele­ment of rig­or­ous treat­ment stud­ies. For each study, the authors pro­vide a detailed cri­tique it’s strengths and lim­i­ta­tions. As a detailed review of the indi­vid­ual study cri­tiques is beyond what I can do here, below I sum­ma­rize the authors’ con­clu­sions on the state of the science.

Results Sum­ma­ry

Treat­ment effects

When aver­aged across the stud­ies for which appro­pri­ate out­come data was avail­able, the over­all mean effect size (ES) was .79 for inat­ten­tion mea­sures, and .71 for hyperactivity/impulsivity mea­sures. These are in a range that would be con­sid­ered ‘large’ for inat­ten­tion and ‘mod­er­ate’ for hyperactivity/impulsivity and are below what is typ­i­cal­ly report­ed for stim­u­lant med­ica­tion. Five of the stud­ies showed neu­ro­phys­i­o­log­i­cal changes that were spe­cif­ic to neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment. Over­all, these results are con­sis­tent with ben­e­fi­cial effects of neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment for ADHD.

Sum­ma­ry of study limitations

The authors iden­ti­fied 5 dif­fer­ent lim­i­ta­tions that under­mine the con­clu­sions about neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment effi­ca­cy that can be made.

1. Min­i­mal use of Triple Blinding 

The ide­al study would be one where chil­dren, par­ents and/or teach­ers who rate chil­dren’s behav­ior before and after treat­ment, and clin­i­cians don’t know whether the child received active treat­ment. This elim­i­nates — or at least strong­ly reduces — the like­li­hood that appar­ent ben­e­fits asso­ci­at­ed with neu­ro­feed­back can be explained by expec­ta­tions that the child would benefit.

Only 4 of the 14 stud­ies uti­lized triple blind pro­ce­dures, how­ev­er, and in 6 of the stud­ies none of these 3 sources was blind.

2. Nature of Con­trol Group 

The strongest neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment study would be one that used ‘sham’ treat­ment for chil­dren ran­dom­ized to the con­trol group, i.e., par­tic­i­pants receive feed­back that is not linked to the EEG state that is the focus of actu­al train­ing. The ben­e­fit of this is that — in the­o­ry — it keeps chil­dren, par­ents, and clin­i­cians blind to whether real treat­ment is being pro­vid­ed, thus elim­i­nat­ing poten­tial bias­es to the out­come rat­ings they provide.

For the 14 stud­ies review, how­ev­er, only 4 employed sham treat­ment. And, of those 4, only 1 used what was felt to be a tru­ly cred­i­ble ‘sham’. In the absence of a cred­i­ble ‘sham’ treat­ment, con­duct­ing a ‘triple blind’ study is not possible.

The oth­er stud­ies either used ‘wait list’ con­trols or com­pared neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment to a dif­fer­ent type of cog­ni­tive train­ing. The use of wait list and alter­na­tive treat­ment con­trol groups are preva­lent in the treat­ment lit­er­a­ture, but are less able than a true ‘sham’ con­di­tion to unequiv­o­cal­ly estab­lish that treat­ment gains asso­ci­at­ed with neu­ro­feed­back are attrib­ut­able to the feed­back chil­dren receive on their EEG state.

3. Insuf­fi­cient iden­ti­fi­ca­tion, mea­sure­ment, and con­trol of con­comi­tant treatments

Chil­dren par­tic­i­pat­ing in these stud­ies were fre­quent­ly receiv­ing oth­er treat­ments as well, either med­ica­tion, psy­chother­a­py, or edu­ca­tion­al inter­ven­tions. Because the pres­ence and changes in con­comi­tant treat­ments tend­ed not to be care­ful­ly mon­i­tored, how­ev­er, pos­i­tive change asso­ci­at­ed with neu­ro­feed­back may have been caused, or at least influ­enced in some way, by unre­port­ed changes in these oth­er treatments.

4. Gen­er­al lack of post-treat­ment follow-up 

Fol­low­ing chil­dren beyond the end of neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment is crit­i­cal for deter­min­ing long-term effi­ca­cy and/or the need for boost­er ses­sions. How­ev­er, only 3 of the stud­ies includ­ed a post-treat­ment fol­low up of neu­ro­feed­back. And, in these stud­ies, the pro­ce­dures for assess­ing the sus­tain­abil­i­ty of treat­ment ben­e­fits were judged to be com­pro­mised. Thus, the authors con­clude that the dura­tion of any gains asso­ci­at­ed with neu­ro­feed­back remains large­ly unknown.

5. Lim­it­ed atten­tion to pos­si­ble adverse side effects

Although neu­ro­feed­back is described as safe and with­out side effects, only 1 study actu­al­ly mon­i­tored and report­ed adverse events that chil­dren and par­ents relat­ed to treat­ment. Although no such effects were found, some have argued that all tru­ly effec­tive treat­ments pro­duce some side effects in some per­cent­age of indi­vid­u­als who receive them. Thus, rather than not attend­ing to this pos­si­bil­i­ty in neu­ro­feed­back stud­ies because the treat­ment is assumed to be safe, the authors sug­gest that this is an area where greater scruti­ny is warranted.

Over­all Summary

Based on their review of the lit­er­a­ture, the authors argue that “…due to the lack of blind­ing and sham con­trol con­di­tions in ran­dom­ized stud­ies” neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment for ADHD should not be con­sid­ered ‘Effi­ca­cious and Spe­cif­ic’ as was con­clud­ed in the 2009 review by Arn and his colleagues.

Instead, they believe that a grade of 3 on the APA evi­dence scale, which cor­re­sponds to ‘Prob­a­bly Effi­ca­cious’ is war­rant­ed. They note that a large mul­ti­site triple-blind sham-con­trolled Ran­dom­ized Con­trolled Tri­al is need­ed to set­tle the issue.

Clear­ly, it is pos­si­ble to review the same evi­dence and reach a dif­fer­ent con­clu­sion. Some would argue that the authors are over­ly cau­tious in the evi­dence grade they assign and that more is being required of neu­ro­feed­back than of oth­er ADHD treat­ments. For exam­ple, although the long-term ben­e­fits of neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment may remain rel­a­tive­ly unknown, evi­dence on the long-terms ben­e­fits of med­ica­tion treat­ment is also limited.

One could also argue that requir­ing a triple-blind tri­al with a cred­i­ble sham con­di­tion is unrea­son­able because this is a high­er stan­dard than that employed most psy­chother­a­py out­come research. In stud­ies to estab­lish the effi­ca­cy of behav­ioral treat­ment for ADHD, for exam­ple, a triple blind tri­al is not pos­si­ble because clin­i­cians know what treat­ment they are pro­vid­ing and par­ents will know what treat­ment their child is receiv­ing. Despite this, behav­ior ther­a­py is con­sid­ered a strong evi­dence-based treat­ment for ADHD.

In response to this objec­tion, the authors argue that the high­est stan­dard of sci­en­tif­ic rig­or should be required for any treat­ment offered to the pub­lic for which triple blind stud­ies are pos­si­ble (they are not pos­si­ble for behav­ior ther­a­py), and which are not pre­clud­ed by strong eth­i­cal con­sid­er­a­tions. They note that this is espe­cial­ly true for neu­ro­feed­back, as such a study is pos­si­ble and the treat­ment requires sub­stan­tial time, effort, and expense.

Some Final Thoughts

My view is con­sis­tent with the authors. I would very much like to see the type of study they call for and believe the evi­dence grade they sug­gest of ‘Prob­a­bly Effi­ca­cious’ is appro­pri­ate. Hav­ing this con­clu­sion pub­lished in a sci­en­tif­ic jour­nal that does not focus on neu­ro­feed­back research rep­re­sents sig­nif­i­cant progress for the field as it was not too long ago that a com­mon­ly held view seemed to be that there was lit­tle if any cred­i­ble evi­dence sup­port­ing this treatment.

It is also impor­tant to rec­og­nize that what remains unclear is not whether chil­dren with ADHD who receive care­ful­ly admin­is­tered neu­ro­feed­back will gen­er­al­ly derive some ben­e­fit — the stud­ies reviewed in this arti­cle estab­lish that — but, rather, why does ben­e­fit occurs. Here is what the authors say:

“…due to the lack of con­trols, it is unclear as to whether the large ESs for impul­siv­i­ty and inat­ten­tion and the medi­um ES for hyper­ac­tiv­i­ty are due to the active com­po­nent of EF and/or non­spe­cif­ic treat­ment factors.”

In oth­er words, the research estab­lish­es that neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment yields ben­e­fits for core ADHD symp­toms but is not clear on what explains those ben­e­fits. Is it the spe­cif­ic feed­back on EEG activ­i­ty and learn­ing to con­trol that activ­i­ty that pro­duces the gains? Or do non­spe­cif­ic fac­tors asso­ci­at­ed with the treat­ment, e.g., expectan­cy effects, clin­i­cian atten­tion, praise for the effort involved, etc., that actu­al­ly accounts for the gains?

This is the impor­tant sci­en­tif­ic ques­tion that remains to be answered. In the mean­time, how­ev­er, the research reviewed here indi­cates that if par­ents obtain high qual­i­ty neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment for their child there is a rea­son­able basis for expect­ing that ben­e­fits will occur. The deci­sion to do so should be made with the knowl­edge that med­ica­tion treat­ment and behav­ioral ther­a­py would be regard­ed as hav­ing stronger research sup­port at this time.

To dis­miss neu­ro­feed­back treat­ment sim­ply as ‘unproven’, how­ev­er, ignores the con­sid­er­able research on this approach that has been con­duct­ed. Help­ing fam­i­lies bet­ter under­stand the strengths and lim­i­ta­tions of this research can enable them to make a bet­ter informed deci­sion about whether to con­sid­er this treat­ment option for their child.

Rabiner_David– Dr. David Rabin­er is a child clin­i­cal psy­chol­o­gist and Direc­tor of Under­grad­u­ate Stud­ies in the Depart­ment of Psy­chol­ogy and Neu­ro­science at Duke Uni­ver­sity. He pub­lishes Atten­tion Research Update, an online newslet­ter that helps par­ents, pro­fes­sion­als, and edu­ca­tors keep up with the lat­est research on ADHD, and teach­es the online course  How to Nav­i­gate Con­ven­tion­al and Com­ple­men­tary ADHD Treat­ments for Healthy Brain Devel­op­ment.

Pre­vi­ous arti­cles by Dr. Rabin­er:

  • New Study Sup­ports Neu­ro­feed­back Treat­ment for ADHD
  • Neurofeedback/ Quan­ti­ta­tive EEG for ADHD diagnosis
  • Self-Reg­u­la­tion and Barkley’s The­o­ry of ADHD

Share this:

  • Tweet
  • Email
  • Print
  • More
  • Share on Tumblr
  • Pock­et

Filed Under: Attention & ADD/ADHD, Brain/ Mental Health Tagged With: ADHD-Treatment, American-Psychological-Association, brainwave, CHADD, EEG, EEG-Biofeedback, hyperactive behavior, impulsive behavior, mental health treatments, multimodal treatment, Neurofeedback, Neurofeedback-Treatment

Reader Interactions

Comments

  1. Dr. L. Steiner says

    September 18, 2016 at 9:10

    Thanks so much for this fair, bal­anced and extreme­ly infor­ma­tive arti­cle. Is there any­thing more cur­rent on this topic?

Primary Sidebar

Top Articles on Brain Health and Neuroplasticity

  1. Can you grow your hippocampus? Yes. Here’s how, and why it matters
  2. How learning changes your brain
  3. To harness neuroplasticity, start with enthusiasm
  4. Three ways to protect your mental health during –and after– COVID-19
  5. Why you turn down the radio when you're lost
  6. Solving the Brain Fitness Puzzle Is the Key to Self-Empowered Aging
  7. Ten neu­rotech­nolo­gies about to trans­form brain enhance­ment & health
  8. Five reasons the future of brain enhancement is digital, pervasive and (hopefully) bright
  9. What Educators and Parents Should Know About Neuroplasticity and Dance
  10. The Ten Habits of Highly Effective Brains
  11. Six tips to build resilience and prevent brain-damaging stress
  12. Can brain training work? Yes, if it meets these 5 conditions
  13. What are cognitive abilities and how to boost them?
  14. Eight Tips To Remember What You Read
  15. Twenty Must-Know Facts to Harness Neuroplasticity and Improve Brain Health

Top 10 Brain Teasers and Illusions

  1. You think you know the colors? Try the Stroop Test
  2. Check out this brief attention experiment
  3. Test your stress level
  4. Guess: Are there more brain connections or leaves in the Amazon?
  5. Quick brain teasers to flex two key men­tal mus­cles
  6. Count the Fs in this sentence
  7. Can you iden­tify Apple’s logo?
  8. Ten classic optical illu­sions to trick your mind
  9. What do you see?
  10. Fun Mental Rotation challenge
  • Check our Top 25 Brain Teasers, Games and Illusions

Join 12,560 readers exploring, at no cost, the latest in neuroplasticity and brain health.

By subscribing you agree to receive our free, monthly eNewsletter. We don't rent or sell emails collected, and you may unsubscribe at any time.

IMPORTANT: Please check your inbox or spam folder in a couple minutes and confirm your subscription.

Get In Touch!

Contact Us

660 4th Street, Suite 205,
San Francisco, CA 94107 USA

About Us

SharpBrains is an independent market research firm tracking health and performance applications of brain science. We prepare general and tailored market reports, publish consumer guides, produce an annual global and virtual conference, and provide strategic advisory services.

© 2023 SharpBrains. All Rights Reserved - Privacy Policy