Sharp Brains: Brain Fitness and Cognitive Health News

Neuroplasticity, Brain Fitness and Cognitive Health News


Build Your Cognitive Reserve: An Interview with Dr. Yaakov Stern

Yaakov SternDr. Yaakov Stern is the Divi­sion Leader of the Cog­ni­tive Neu­ro­science Divi­sion of the Sergievsky Cen­ter, and Pro­fes­sor of Clin­i­cal Neu­ropsy­chol­o­gy, at the Col­lege of Physi­cians and Sur­geons of Colum­bia Uni­ver­si­ty, New York. Alvaro Fer­nan­dez inter­views him here as part of our research for The Sharp­Brains Guide to Brain Fit­ness book.

Dr. Stern is one of the lead­ing pro­po­nents of the Cog­ni­tive reserve the­o­ry, which aims to explain why some indi­vid­u­als with full Alzheimer’s pathol­o­gy (accu­mu­la­tion of plaques and tan­gles in their brains) can keep nor­mal lives until they die, while oth­ers -with the same amount of plaques and tan­gles- dis­play the severe symp­toms we asso­ciate with Alzheimer’s Dis­ease. He has pub­lished dozens of peer-reviewed sci­en­tif­ic papers on the sub­ject.

The con­cept of a Cog­ni­tive Reserve has been around since 1989, when a post mortem analy­sis of 137 peo­ple with Alzheimer’s Dis­ease showed that some patients exhib­it­ed few­er clin­i­cal symp­toms than their actu­al pathol­o­gy sug­gest­ed. These patients also showed high­er brain weights and greater num­ber of neu­rons when com­pared to age-matched con­trols. The inves­ti­ga­tors hypoth­e­sized that the patients had a larg­er “reserve” of neu­rons and abil­i­ties that enable them to off­set the loss­es caused by Alzheimer’s. Since then, the con­cept of Cog­ni­tive Reserve has been defined as the abil­i­ty of an indi­vid­ual to tol­er­ate pro­gres­sive brain pathol­o­gy with­out demon­strat­ing clin­i­cal cog­ni­tive symp­toms. (You can check at the end of this inter­view a great clip on this).


Key take-aways

- Life­time expe­ri­ences, like edu­ca­tion, engag­ing occu­pa­tion, and leisure activ­i­ties, have been shown to have a major influ­ence on how we age, specif­i­cal­ly on whether we will devel­op Alzheimer’s symp­toms or not.

- This is so because stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties, ide­al­ly com­bin­ing phys­i­cal exer­cise, learn­ing and social inter­ac­tion, help us build a Cog­ni­tive Reserve to pro­tect us.

- The ear­li­er we start build­ing our Reserve, the bet­ter; but it is nev­er too late to start. And, the more activ­i­ties, the bet­ter: the effect is cumu­la­tive.


The Cog­ni­tive Reserve

Alvaro Fer­nan­dez (AF): Dear Dr. Stern, it is a plea­sure to have you here. Let me first ask you this: the impli­ca­tions of your research are pret­ty broad, pre­sent­ing major impli­ca­tions across sec­tors and age groups. What has been the most unex­pect­ed reac­tion so far?

YS: well…I was pret­ty sur­prised when, years ago, a reporter from Sev­en­teen mag­a­zine request­ed an inter­view. I was real­ly curi­ous to learn why she felt that her read­ers would be inter­est­ed in stud­ies about demen­tia. What she told me showed a deep under­stand­ing and insight: she want­ed to moti­vate chil­dren to stay in school. She under­stood that ear­ly social inter­ven­tions could be very pow­er­ful for build­ing reserve and pre­vent­ing demen­tia.

AF: Very insightful…let’s now fast for­ward, say, 60 years from our high-school years, and sup­pose that per­sons A and B both tech­ni­cal­ly have Alzheimer’s (plaques and tan­gles appear in the brain), but only A is show­ing the dis­ease symp­toms. What may explain this dis­crep­an­cy?

YS: Indi­vid­u­als who lead men­tal­ly stim­u­lat­ing lives, through edu­ca­tion, occu­pa­tion and leisure activ­i­ties, have reduced risk of devel­op­ing Alzheimer’s. Stud­ies sug­gest that they have 35–40% less risk of man­i­fest­ing the dis­ease. The pathol­o­gy will still occur, but they are able to cope with it bet­ter. Some won’t ever be diag­nosed with Alzheimer’s because they don’t present any symp­toms. In stud­ies that fol­low healthy elders over time and then get autop­sies, up to 20% of peo­ple who did not present any sig­nif­i­cant prob­lem in the dai­ly lives have full blown Alzheimer’s pathol­o­gy in their brains.

AF: What exact­ly may be going on in the brain that pro­vides that lev­el of pro­tec­tion?

YS: There are two ideas that are com­ple­men­tary. One idea (called Brain Reserve by researchers) pos­tu­lates that some indi­vid­u­als have a greater num­ber of neu­rons and synaps­es, and that some­how those extra struc­tures pro­vide a lev­el of pro­tec­tion. In a sense, we have more “hard­ware”, pro­vid­ing a pas­sive pro­tec­tion against the attacks of Alzheimer’s. The oth­er the­o­ry (called Cog­ni­tive Reserve) empha­sizes the build­ing of new capa­bil­i­ties, how peo­ple can per­form tasks bet­ter through prac­tice, and how these skills become so well learned that they are not too easy to unlearn. Like devel­op­ing new and refined “soft­ware”.

AF: But, both seem to go hand in hand, cor­rect? Neu­ro­plas­tic­i­ty means that what you call “hard­ware” and “soft­ware” are two sides of the same coin and they influ­ence each oth­er, right?

YS: Cor­rect. So these days we don’t make a sharp dis­tinc­tion, and are con­duct­ing more neu­roimag­ing stud­ies to bet­ter under­stand the rela­tion­ship between both.

Build­ing Your Cog­ni­tive Reserve

AF: So our goal is to build that Reserve of neu­rons, synaps­es, and skills. How can we do that? What defines “men­tal­ly stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties” or good “brain exer­cise”?

YS: In sum­ma­ry, we could say that “stim­u­la­tion” con­sists of engag­ing in activ­i­ties. In our research almost all activ­i­ties are seen to con­tribute to reserve. Some have chal­leng­ing lev­els of cog­ni­tive com­plex­i­ty, and some have inter­per­son­al or phys­i­cal demands. In ani­mal stud­ies, expo­sure to an enriched envi­ron­ment or increased phys­i­cal activ­i­ty result in increased neu­ro­ge­n­e­sis (the cre­ation of new neu­rons). You can get that stim­u­la­tion through edu­ca­tion and/ or your occu­pa­tion. There is clear research show­ing how those two ele­ments reduce the risk. Now, what is very excit­ing is that, no mat­ter one’s age, edu­ca­tion and occu­pa­tion, our lev­el of par­tic­i­pa­tion in leisure activ­i­ties has a sig­nif­i­cant and cumu­la­tive effect. A key mes­sage here is that dif­fer­ent activ­i­ties have inde­pen­dent, syn­er­gis­tic, con­tri­bu­tions, which means the more things you do and the ear­li­er you start, the bet­ter. But you are nev­er stuck: bet­ter late than nev­er.

AF: Can you give us some exam­ples of those leisure activ­i­ties that seem to have the most pos­i­tive effects?

YS: For our 2001 study we eval­u­at­ed the effect of 13 activ­i­ties, com­bin­ing intel­lec­tu­al, phys­i­cal, and social ele­ments. Some of the activ­i­ties with the most effect were read­ing, vis­it­ing friends or rel­a­tives, going to movies or restau­rants, and walk­ing for plea­sure or going on an excur­sion. As you can see, a vari­ety. We saw that the group with high lev­el of leisure activ­i­ties pre­sent­ed 38% less risk (con­trol­ling for oth­er fac­tors) of devel­op­ing Alzheimer’s symp­toms. And that, for each addi­tion­al type of activ­i­ty, the risk got reduced by 8%. There is an addi­tion­al ele­ment that we are start­ing to see more clear­ly. Phys­i­cal exer­cise, by itself, also has a very ben­e­fi­cial impact on cog­ni­tion. Only a few months ago researchers were able to show for the first time how phys­i­cal activ­i­ty pro­motes neu­ro­ge­n­e­sis in the human brain. So, we need both men­tal and phys­i­cal exer­cise. The not-so-good news is that, as of today, there no clear recipe for suc­cess. More research is need­ed before we pre­pare a sys­tem­at­ic set of inter­ven­tions that can help max­i­mize our pro­tec­tion.

AF: We typ­i­cal­ly empha­size the impor­tance of a good nutri­tion, phys­i­cal exer­cise, stress man­age­ment and men­tal exer­cise that presents nov­el­ty, vari­ety and chal­lenge. What do you think of the rel­a­tive­ly recent appear­ance of so many com­put­er-based cog­ni­tive train­ing pro­grams, some more sci­ence-based than oth­ers?

YS: Those ele­ments you men­tion make sense. The prob­lem is that, at least from the point of view of Alzheimer’s, we can­not be much more spe­cif­ic. We don’t know if learn­ing a new lan­guage is more ben­e­fi­cial than learn­ing a new musi­cal instru­ment or using a com­put­er-based pro­gram. A few of the cog­ni­tive train­ing com­put­er pro­grams we have seen, like the one you dis­cussed with Prof. Daniel Gopher to train the men­tal abil­i­ties of pilots, seem to have clear effects on cog­ni­tion, gen­er­al­iz­ing beyond the train­ing itself. But, for the most part, it is too ear­ly to tell the long-term effects. We need bet­ter designed clin­i­cal tri­als with clear con­trols. Right now, the most we can say is that those who lead men­tal­ly stim­u­lat­ing lives, through edu­ca­tion, occu­pa­tion and leisure activ­i­ties seem to have the least risk of devel­op­ing Alzheimer’s Dis­ease.

Research inter­ests

AF: Tell us know a bit more about your cur­rent research

YS: We are study­ing a num­ber of relat­ed areas, apply­ing neu­roimag­ing tech­niques to under­stand how exact­ly all these Cog­ni­tive Reserve con­cepts are imple­ment­ed in the brain.

One, we want to under­stand indi­vid­ual dif­fer­ences in how peo­ple approach tasks. We want to mea­sure their effi­cien­cy and capac­i­ty the brain net­works that medi­ate tasks per­for­mance with the idea that those with greater effi­cien­cy and capac­i­ty might cope bet­ter with age-relat­ed prob­lems. For exam­ple, we can all under­stand that a com­pet­i­tive swim­mer is going to swim bet­ter than I would even if he has some weights in his legs, but we haven’t yet iden­ti­fied what exact­ly is the equiv­a­lent in the brain.

Sec­ond, we want to under­stand how old peo­ple com­pen­sate for the areas of decline. For exam­ple, do they begin to use new brain areas when the ones that are typ­i­cal­ly used start to fail.

Third, whether the Cog­ni­tive Reserve presents ben­e­fits beyond the pre­ven­tion of Alzheimer’s symp­toms. Does hav­ing a high­er reserve result in bet­ter atten­tion, bet­ter exec­u­tive func­tions, more suc­cess­ful aging over­all?

AF: All very impor­tant top­ics. And I am sure every­one read­ing this inter­view will devour any new details on how to build our Cog­ni­tive Reserves. Thank you for your time, and please keep us informed.

YS: My plea­sure. Thank you for your great edu­ca­tion­al ini­tia­tive.


Sci­en­tif­ic ref­er­ences

Stern Y. Cog­ni­tive reserve and Alzheimer dis­ease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Dis­ord. 2006, 20:112–117.

Scarmeas N, Albert SM, Man­ly J, Stern Y. Edu­ca­tion and rates of cog­ni­tive decline in inci­dent Alzheimer’s dis­ease. Jour­nal of Neu­rol­o­gy, Neu­ro­surgery, and Psy­chi­a­try, 2006;77:308–316.

Scarmeas N, Stern Y. Cog­ni­tive reserve and lifestyle. J Clin Exp Neu­ropsy­chol 2003;5:625–633.

Stern Y, Zarahn E, Hilton HJ, Fly­nn J, DeLa Paz R, Rak­itin B. Explor­ing the neur­al basis of cog­ni­tive reserve. J Clin Exp Neu­ropsy­chol 2003;5:691–701.

Leave a Reply...

Loading Facebook Comments ...

34 Responses

  1. Kim says:

    What a great inter­view! And the video at the end was very inspir­ing!

  2. Gary D says:

    Absolute­ly fas­ci­nat­ing. I nev­er thought about brain reserve. The nun video was awe­some! It real­ly got me moti­vat­ed to get back to exer­cise. Not just my body, but my brain too. Thanks for post­ing this infor­ma­tion!!

Leave a Reply

Categories: Cognitive Neuroscience, Health & Wellness, Neuroscience Interview Series

Tags: , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , ,

All Slidedecks & Recordings Available — click image below

Search for anything brain-related in our article archives

About SharpBrains

As seen in The New York Times, The Wall Street Journal, BBC News, CNN, Reuters, and more, SharpBrains is an independent market research firm and think tank tracking health and performance applications of brain science.