Study: Computerized skills-training can reduce inattentive driving and collisions in teens with ADHD
Motor vehicle collisions are a leading cause of death among teens, and teens with ADHD are about twice as likely to be in a collision. Teens with ADHD are more likely to look away from the road for long periods when performing distracting tasks, e.g., viewing their phones, checking a map, etc., as opposed to taking repeated brief glances. This increases the risk of collision, suggesting that training teens with ADHD to reduce this behavior could reduce their driving risk.
A recent study published in the New England Journal of Medicine examines whether training teens with ADHD to reduce their long glances away from the road would reduce their collision risk: Trial of training to reduce driver inattention in teens with ADHD. Participants where 152 16–19 year-old teens with ADHD and a valid drivers license and who spent at least 3 hours per week in unsupervised driving.
The Study:
Teens were randomly assigned to the computerized training condition or a control condition. In the training condition, which included 5 90-minute sessions, teens completed ‘drives’ in a driving simulator. An eye-tracking device on the dashboard monitored when their eyes were directed towards the simulated roadway and when they were looking elsewhere. During the simulated drives, they were required to perform several distracting tasks, including searching for street names on a map, and having to identify specific symbols on a computerized dashboard display.
During training, the teens received auditory feedback via whenever the eye tracker detected their eyes remaining off the roadway for a set period of time, initially 3 seconds and then reduced to 2 seconds. The goal of this feedback was to increase their awareness of long glances away from the road and to ultimately reduce this behavior in the absence of feedback.
Participants assigned to the control condition included a typical drivers education curriculum along with similar time in the driving simulator performing the same tasks as intervention participants. However, unlike those in the intervention group, teens in the control condition received no auditory feedback when their eyes were focused on the task and not the road for an extended period. This is an ideal control condition as these participants were treated similarly to intervention youth except for the feedback.
Measured Outcomes:
There were 2 primary outcomes in this trial. The first was the number of long glances (lasting over 2 seconds) away from the roadway during two 15-minute simulated drives that were conducted at baseline and at 1 month and 6 months after training. During these simulated drives, participants were required to perform a task that would distract their eyes from the road.
The second primary outcome was the standard deviation of lane position, as measured in feet, during the simulator drives. This outcome measured the degree to which participants ‘cars’ deviated from the mid-point of the lane, with higher values indicating greater deviation. Essentially, it reflects the extent to which participants tended to swerve while driving.
Secondary outcomes were collected during actual on-road driving for 12 months after training ended. Through accelerometers and eye-tracking devices installed in participants’ cars, researchers tracked the number of sudden acceleration or deceleration events, i.e., g‑force events. They could determine whether a long-glance away from the road accompanied these events, and whether they involved a collision or near collision. These real-world and important driving outcomes enable to test whether training reduced real-world negative driving behavior and outcomes.
Results:
At one month after training, teens in the intervention group had over 40% fewer long glances away from the road than control participants, 16.5 versus 28.0. Similar results were found at 6 months, 15.7 vs. 27 long glances.
Teens receiving training also showed significantly fewer deviations from the midpoint of the lane than control participants at both 1 month and 6 months, thus indicating that they more consistently stayed in their lane.
The real world driving outcomes also favored teens who received training. Although they averaged more acceleration/deceleration events than control participants, 37.1 vs. 42.3, these events were less likely to occur during a long glance away from the road, 18.3% vs. 23.9%
And, most importantly, actual collisions or near collisions occurred in only 3.4% of excessive acceleration/deceleration events for the intervention group versus 5.6% among teens in the control group. Furthermore, the actual number of collisions or near collisions was 110 in the intervention group compared to 159 among teens in the control group, a difference of nearly 25%.
Discussion:
Among teens with ADHD distracted during simulated driving, training them to reduce the length of glances away from the road during distracting tasks was found to have a positive impact.
During simulated driving, teens who received training took fewer long glances away from the road at both 1‑month and 6‑month followups. And, most importantly, the number of collisions and near collisions they experienced in the year following training was also reduced by approximately 25%. It is likely that this occurred because training resulted in their taking fewer long glances away from the road.
These are important findings given the dangers of distracted driving in teens with ADHD. And, although medication treatment also has been shown to improve driving behavior in teens with ADHD, even those who take medication are often driving at times when it would not be active in their system.
Let’s hope that this approach to reducing driving risk in teens with ADHD is something that can be made accessible to teens and their families. The study is available here and includes interesting videos demonstrating some of the methods that were used.
– Dr. David Rabiner is a child clinical psychologist and Director of Undergraduate Studies in the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience at Duke University. He publishes the Attention Research Update, an online newsletter that helps parents, professionals, and educators keep up with the latest research on ADHD.