New study links motor-sensory biases with cognitive and social abilities

Whether you’re left, right or ambidex­trous, “hand­ed­ness” is part of our iden­ti­ty. But a lot of peo­ple don’t realise that we have oth­er bias­es too and they are not unique to humans. My col­leagues and I have pub­lished a new study that shows align­ing our bias­es in the same way as oth­er peo­ple may have social benefits.

Behavioural biases are not unique to humans

Across dif­fer­ent cul­tures, human pop­u­la­tions have high lev­els of right-hand­ed­ness (around 90%). We also have a strong pop­u­la­tion bias in how we recog­nise faces and their emotions.

A sig­nif­i­cant major­i­ty of the pop­u­la­tion are faster and more accu­rate at recog­nis­ing iden­ti­ties and emo­tions when they fall with­in the left visu­al field com­pared with the right visu­al field.

These types of bias­es devel­op in our brains in ear­ly child­hood. The left and right hemi­spheres of the brain con­trol motor action on the oppo­site sides of the body. If your left visu­al field is dom­i­nant, that means the right side of your brain is tak­ing dom­i­nance for recog­nis­ing faces and emotions.

Until recent­ly, sci­en­tists thought behav­iour­al bias­es were unique to humans. But ani­mal research over the last sev­er­al decades shows there are behav­iour­al bias­es across all branch­es of the ver­te­brate tree of life.

For exam­ple, chicks that peck for food with an eye bias are bet­ter at telling grain from peb­bles. Also, chicks with an eye bias for mon­i­tor­ing preda­tors are less like­ly to be eat­en than unlat­er­alised chicks. Stud­ies show that ani­mals with bias­es tend to per­form bet­ter at sur­vival-relat­ed tasks in lab­o­ra­to­ry exper­i­ments, which prob­a­bly trans­lates to a bet­ter sur­vival rate in the wild.

But the chicks with the best advan­tage are ones that favour one eye to the ground (to find food) and the oth­er eye to the sky (to look out for threats). A ben­e­fit of the “divid­ed brain” is that wild ani­mals can for­age for food and look out for preda­tors – impor­tant multitasking.

Why do animals have behavioural biases?

Research sug­gests that brain hemi­sphere bias­es evolved because it allows the two sides of the brain to con­cur­rent­ly con­trol dif­fer­ent behav­iour. It also pro­tects ani­mals from becom­ing mud­dled. If both sides of the brain had equal con­trol over crit­i­cal func­tions they might simul­ta­ne­ous­ly direct the body to car­ry out incom­pat­i­ble responses.

So bias­es free up some resources or “neur­al capac­i­ty”, mak­ing ani­mals more effi­cient at find­ing food and keep­ing safe from predators.

Ani­mal stud­ies sug­gest it is the pres­ence, not the direc­tion (left or right) of our bias­es that mat­ters for per­for­mance. But that doesn’t explain why so many peo­ple are right-hand­ed for motor tasks and left visu­al field biased for face processing.

Every per­son should have a 50–50 chance of being left or right biased. Yet across the ani­mal king­dom, the major­i­ty of indi­vid­u­als in a species align in the same direction.

This sug­gests that align­ing bias­es with oth­ers in your group might have a social advan­tage. For exam­ple, ani­mals that align with the pop­u­la­tion dur­ing coop­er­a­tive behav­iour (shoal­ing, flock­ing) dilute the pos­si­bil­i­ty of being picked off by a preda­tor. The few that turn away from the flock or shoal become clear targets.

Although humans are high­ly lat­er­alised regard­less of eth­nic or geo­graph­ic back­ground, there is always a sig­nif­i­cant minor­i­ty in the pop­u­la­tion, sug­gest­ing that this alter­na­tive bias has its own merits.

The pre­vail­ing the­o­ry is that devi­at­ing from the pop­u­la­tion offers ani­mals an advan­tage dur­ing com­pet­i­tive inter­ac­tions, by cre­at­ing an ele­ment of sur­prise. It may explain why left-hand­ed­ness is over-rep­re­sent­ed in pro­fes­sion­al inter­ac­tive sports like crick­et and baseball.

New study linking motor-sensory biases with cognitive and social abilities

In the first study of its kind, sci­en­tists from the uni­ver­si­ties of Sus­sex, Oxford, West­min­ster, Lon­don (City, Birk­beck) and Kent put our human behav­iour­al bias­es to the test. We inves­ti­gat­ed asso­ci­a­tions between strength of hand bias and per­for­mance as well as direc­tion of bias­es and social abil­i­ty. We chose behav­iour that aligns with ani­mal research.

Over 1,600 peo­ple of all ages and eth­nic­i­ties par­tic­i­pat­ed in this investigation.

You don’t always use your pre­ferred hand: some peo­ple are mild­ly, mod­er­ate­ly or strong­ly hand­ed. So we mea­sured hand­ed­ness in our par­tic­i­pants using a timed colour-match­ing peg­board task. Not every­one knows whether they have a visu­al field bias so we eval­u­at­ed this for par­tic­i­pants using images of faces express­ing dif­fer­ent emo­tions (such as anger and sur­prise) pre­sent­ed on a screen.

Peo­ple with mild to mod­er­ate strength hand bias (left or right) placed more colour-matched pegs cor­rect­ly than those with a strong or weak bias. These results sug­gest that, in humans, extremes may lim­it our per­for­mance flex­i­bil­i­ty, unlike wild animals.

The major­i­ty of the par­tic­i­pants had a stan­dard bias (right hand­ed­ness for motor tasks, left visu­al field bias for face pro­cess­ing). But not everyone.

To test the asso­ci­a­tions of social skills and bias direc­tion, par­tic­i­pants were cat­e­gorised by their hand and visu­al side bias­es into one of four groups: stan­dard (right hand, left visu­al), crowd­ed right (right hand, right visu­al), crowd­ed left (left hand, left visu­al) and reversed (left hand, right visu­al). They also com­plet­ed a sur­vey that eval­u­at­ed their social difficulties.

The stan­dard pro­file, found in 53% of par­tic­i­pants, was not asso­ci­at­ed with a social advan­tage over crowd­ed left or right groups. How­ev­er, the reversed pro­file, which was rel­a­tive­ly rare (12%), was asso­ci­at­ed with sig­nif­i­cant­ly low­er social scores com­pared with the oth­er groups. Peo­ple in the reversed group were four times more like­ly to have a self-report­ed diag­no­sis of autism or atten­tion deficit hyper­ac­tiv­i­ty dis­or­der (ADHD).

We can­not say from this study whether there is a causal rela­tion­ship between the reversed pro­file and autism and ADHD. How­ev­er, we are plan­ning research to inves­ti­gate if bias pro­files can act as an ear­ly risk mark­er for autism and ADHD dur­ing infan­cy, which could pave a way for ear­li­er screen­ing, diag­no­sis and the devel­op­ment of new interventions.

This study is a reminder that we humans have an evo­lu­tion­ary his­to­ry, much of which we share with oth­er ani­mals. We need to study our­selves with­in the con­text of the wider ani­mal king­dom if we want to tru­ly under­stand our mod­ern brains and behaviour.

Gillian For­rester is a Pro­fes­sor of Com­par­a­tive Cog­ni­tion at the Uni­ver­si­ty of Sus­sex, where she research­es human and non-human pri­mate cog­ni­tion. This arti­cle was orig­i­nal­ly pub­lished on The Con­ver­sa­tion.

To Learn More:

Leave a Reply

About SharpBrains

SHARPBRAINS is an independent think-tank and consulting firm providing services at the frontier of applied neuroscience, health, leadership and innovation.
SHARPBRAINS es un think-tank y consultoría independiente proporcionando servicios para la neurociencia aplicada, salud, liderazgo e innovación.

Top Articles on Brain Health and Neuroplasticity

Top 10 Brain Teasers and Illusions

Newsletter

Subscribe to our e-newsletter

* indicates required

Got the book?