• Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar

Tracking Health and Wellness Applications of Brain Science

Spanish
sb-logo-with-brain
  • Resources
    • Monthly eNewsletter
    • Solving the Brain Fitness Puzzle
    • The SharpBrains Guide to Brain Fitness
    • How to evaluate brain training claims
    • Resources at a Glance
  • Brain Teasers
    • Top 25 Brain Teasers & Games for Teens and Adults
    • Brain Teasers for each Cognitive Ability
    • More Mind Teasers & Games for Adults of any Age
  • Virtual Summits
    • 2019 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
    • Speaker Roster
    • Brainnovations Pitch Contest
    • 2017 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
    • 2016 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
    • 2015 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
    • 2014 SharpBrains Virtual Summit
  • Report: Pervasive Neurotechnology
  • Report: Digital Brain Health
  • About
    • Mission & Team
    • Endorsements
    • Public Speaking
    • In the News
    • Contact Us

Study finds that playing videogames may be more cognitively beneficial for children than other forms of screentime (social media, watching videos/ TV)

June 1, 2022 by The Conversation

Many par­ents feel guilty when their chil­dren play video games for hours on end. Some even wor­ry it could make their chil­dren less clever. And, indeed, that’s a top­ic sci­en­tists have clashed over for years.

In our new study, we inves­ti­gat­ed how video games affect the minds of chil­dren, inter­view­ing and test­ing more than 5,000 chil­dren aged ten to 12. And the results, pub­lished in Sci­en­tif­ic Reports, will be sur­pris­ing to some.

Chil­dren were asked how many hours a day they spent on social media, watch­ing videos or TV, and play­ing video games. The answer was: a lot of hours. On aver­age, chil­dren spent two and a half hours a day watch­ing online videos or TV pro­grammes, half an hour social­is­ing online, and one hour play­ing video games.

In total, that’s four hours a day for the aver­age child and six hours for the top 25% – a large por­tion of a child’s free time. And oth­er reports found that this has increased dra­mat­i­cal­ly over the decades. Screens were around in pre­vi­ous gen­er­a­tions, but now they tru­ly define childhood.

Is that a bad thing? Well, it’s com­pli­cat­ed. There could be both ben­e­fits and draw­backs for the devel­op­ing minds of chil­dren. And these might depend on the out­come you are look­ing at. For our study, we were specif­i­cal­ly inter­est­ed in the effect of screen time on intel­li­gence – the abil­i­ty to learn effec­tive­ly, think ratio­nal­ly, under­stand com­plex ideas, and adapt to new situations.

Intel­li­gence is an impor­tant trait in our lives and high­ly pre­dic­tive of a child’s future income, hap­pi­ness and longevi­ty. In research, it’s often mea­sured as per­for­mance on a wide range of cog­ni­tive tests. For our study, we cre­at­ed an intel­li­gence index from five tasks: two on read­ing com­pre­hen­sion and vocab­u­lary, one on atten­tion and exec­u­tive func­tion (which includes work­ing mem­o­ry, flex­i­ble think­ing and self-con­trol), one assess­ing visu­al-spa­tial pro­cess­ing (such as rotat­ing objects in your mind), and one on learn­ing abil­i­ty over mul­ti­ple trials.

This is not the first time some­one has stud­ied the effect of screens on intel­li­gence, but research, so far, has pro­duced mixed results. So, what’s spe­cial this time? The nov­el­ty of our study is that we took genes and socioe­co­nom­ic back­grounds into account. Only a few stud­ies so far have con­sid­ered socioe­co­nom­ic sta­tus (house­hold income, parental edu­ca­tion and neigh­bour­hood qual­i­ty), and no study had account­ed for genet­ic effects.

Genes mat­ter because intel­li­gence is high­ly her­i­ta­ble. If unac­count­ed for, these fac­tors could mask the true effect of screen time on children’s intel­li­gence. For exam­ple, chil­dren born with cer­tain genes might be more prone to watch TV and, inde­pen­dent­ly, have learn­ing issues. The lot­tery of genet­ics is a major con­founder in any psy­cho­log­i­cal process, but until recent­ly this has been hard to account for in sci­en­tif­ic stud­ies due to the heavy costs of genome analy­sis and tech­no­log­i­cal limitations.

The data we used for our study is part of a mas­sive data col­lec­tion effort in the US to bet­ter under­stand child­hood devel­op­ment: the Ado­les­cent Brain and Cog­ni­tive Devel­op­ment project. Our sam­ple was rep­re­sen­ta­tive of the US in terms of sex, race, eth­nic­i­ty and socioe­co­nom­ic status.

The results:

We found that when we first asked the child at age ten how much they played, both watch­ing videos and social­is­ing online were linked to below-aver­age intel­li­gence. Mean­while, gam­ing wasn’t linked with intel­li­gence at all. These results of screen time are most­ly in line with pre­vi­ous research. But when we fol­lowed up at a lat­er date, we found that gam­ing had a pos­i­tive and mean­ing­ful effect on intelligence.

While chil­dren who played more video games at ten years were on aver­age no more intel­li­gent than chil­dren who didn’t game, they showed the most gains in intel­li­gence after two years, in both boys and girls. For exam­ple, a child who was in the top 17% in terms of hours spent gam­ing increased their IQ about 2.5 points more than the aver­age child over two years.

This is evi­dence of a ben­e­fi­cial, causal effect of video games on intel­li­gence. This result fits with pre­vi­ous, small­er stud­ies, where par­tic­i­pants are ran­dom­ly assigned to video-game play­ing or a con­trol group. Our find­ing is also in line with par­al­lel lines of stud­ies sug­gest­ing that cog­ni­tive abil­i­ties aren’t fixed, but can be trained – includ­ing stud­ies with cog­ni­tive train­ing inter­ven­tion apps.

What about the oth­er two types of screen activ­i­ties? Social media did not effect the change in intel­li­gence after two years. The many hours of insta­gram­ming and mes­sag­ing did not boost children’s intel­li­gence, but it was not detri­men­tal either. Final­ly, watch­ing TV and online videos showed a pos­i­tive effect in one of the analy­ses, but no effect when parental edu­ca­tion was tak­en into account (as opposed to the broad­er fac­tor of “socioe­co­nom­ic sta­tus”). So this find­ing should be tak­en with a grain of salt. There is some empir­i­cal sup­port that high-qual­i­ty TV/video con­tent, such as the pro­gramme Sesame Street, has a pos­i­tive effect on children’s school per­for­mance and cog­ni­tive abil­i­ties. But those results are rare.

When think­ing about the impli­ca­tions of these find­ings, it is impor­tant to keep in mind that there are many oth­er psy­cho­log­i­cal aspects that we didn’t look at, such as men­tal health, sleep qual­i­ty and phys­i­cal exer­cise. Our results should not be tak­en as a blan­ket rec­om­men­da­tion for all par­ents to allow lim­it­less gam­ing. But for those par­ents both­ered by their chil­dren play­ing video games, you can now feel bet­ter know­ing that it’s prob­a­bly mak­ing them a tad smarter.

– Torkel Kling­berg is a Pro­fes­sor of Cog­ni­tive Neu­ro­science at Karolin­s­ka Insti­tutet and the chief sci­en­tif­ic offi­cer at Cogmed. Bruno Sauce is an Assis­tant Pro­fes­sor of Bio­log­i­cal Psy­chol­o­gy at Vri­je Uni­ver­siteit Ams­ter­dam. This arti­cle was orig­i­nal­ly pub­lished on The Con­ver­sa­tion.

The Study:

The impact of dig­i­tal media on children’s intel­li­gence while con­trol­ling for genet­ic dif­fer­ences in cog­ni­tion and socioe­co­nom­ic back­ground (Sci­en­tif­ic Reports). From the Abstract:

  • Dig­i­tal media defines mod­ern child­hood, but its cog­ni­tive effects are unclear and hot­ly debat­ed. We believe that stud­ies with genet­ic data could clar­i­fy causal claims and cor­rect for the typ­i­cal­ly unac­count­ed role of genet­ic pre­dis­po­si­tions. Here, we esti­mat­ed the impact of dif­fer­ent types of screen time (watch­ing, social­iz­ing, or gam­ing) on children’s intel­li­gence while con­trol­ling for the con­found­ing effects of genet­ic dif­fer­ences in cog­ni­tion and socioe­co­nom­ic sta­tus … Broad­ly, our results are in line with research on the mal­leabil­i­ty of cog­ni­tive abil­i­ties from envi­ron­men­tal fac­tors, such as cog­ni­tive train­ing and the Fly­nn effect.

The Study in Context:

  • What are cog­ni­tive abil­i­ties and how to boost them?
  • Can brain train­ing work? Yes, if it meets these 5 conditions

Share this:

  • Tweet
  • Email
  • Print
  • More
  • Share on Tumblr
  • Pock­et

Filed Under: Brain/ Mental Health, Technology & Innovation Tagged With: cognition, cognitive, cognitive-abilities, cognitive-development, Cognitive-tests, Cognitive-Training, digital media, executive-function, flexible thinking, free time, intelligence, Learning, learning ability, screen time, self-control, social-media, video-games, visual-spatial processing, watching TV, Working-memory

Primary Sidebar

Top Articles on Brain Health and Neuroplasticity

  1. Can you grow your hippocampus? Yes. Here’s how, and why it matters
  2. How learning changes your brain
  3. To harness neuroplasticity, start with enthusiasm
  4. Three ways to protect your mental health during –and after– COVID-19
  5. Why you turn down the radio when you're lost
  6. Solving the Brain Fitness Puzzle Is the Key to Self-Empowered Aging
  7. Ten neu­rotech­nolo­gies about to trans­form brain enhance­ment & health
  8. Five reasons the future of brain enhancement is digital, pervasive and (hopefully) bright
  9. What Educators and Parents Should Know About Neuroplasticity and Dance
  10. The Ten Habits of Highly Effective Brains
  11. Six tips to build resilience and prevent brain-damaging stress
  12. Can brain training work? Yes, if it meets these 5 conditions
  13. What are cognitive abilities and how to boost them?
  14. Eight Tips To Remember What You Read
  15. Twenty Must-Know Facts to Harness Neuroplasticity and Improve Brain Health

Top 10 Brain Teasers and Illusions

  1. You think you know the colors? Try the Stroop Test
  2. Check out this brief attention experiment
  3. Test your stress level
  4. Guess: Are there more brain connections or leaves in the Amazon?
  5. Quick brain teasers to flex two key men­tal mus­cles
  6. Count the Fs in this sentence
  7. Can you iden­tify Apple’s logo?
  8. Ten classic optical illu­sions to trick your mind
  9. What do you see?
  10. Fun Mental Rotation challenge
  • Check our Top 25 Brain Teasers, Games and Illusions

Join 12,563 readers exploring, at no cost, the latest in neuroplasticity and brain health.

By subscribing you agree to receive our free, monthly eNewsletter. We don't rent or sell emails collected, and you may unsubscribe at any time.

IMPORTANT: Please check your inbox or spam folder in a couple minutes and confirm your subscription.

Get In Touch!

Contact Us

660 4th Street, Suite 205,
San Francisco, CA 94107 USA

About Us

SharpBrains is an independent market research firm tracking health and performance applications of brain science. We prepare general and tailored market reports, publish consumer guides, produce an annual global and virtual conference, and provide strategic advisory services.

© 2023 SharpBrains. All Rights Reserved - Privacy Policy