Sharp Brains: Brain Fitness and Cognitive Health News

Neuroplasticity, Brain Fitness and Cognitive Health News


Cognitive Reserve and Lifestyle

Update: we now have an in-depth inter­view with Yaakov Stern, lead­ing advo­cate of the cog­ni­tive reserve the­o­ry, and one of the authors of the paper we review below: click on Build Your Cog­ni­tive Reserve-Yaakov Stern. 


In hon­or of the Week of Sci­ence pre­sent­ed at Just Sci­ence from Mon­day, Feb­ru­ary 5, through Sun­day, Feb­ru­ary 11, we will be writ­ing about “just sci­ence” this week. We thought we would take this time to dis­cuss more deeply some of the key sci­en­tif­ic pub­li­ca­tions in brain fit­ness.

Today, we will high­light the key points in an excel­lent review of cog­ni­tive reserve: Scarmeas, Niko­laos and Stern, Yaakov. Cog­ni­tive reserve and lifestyle. Jour­nal of Clin­i­cal and Exper­i­men­tal Neu­ropsy­chol­o­gy. 2003;25:625–33.

What is Cog­ni­tive Reserve?
The con­cept of a cog­ni­tive reserve has been around since 1998 when a post mortem analy­sis of 137 peo­ple with Alzheimer’s Dis­ease showed that the patients exhib­it­ed few­er clin­i­cal symp­toms than their actu­al pathol­o­gy sug­gest­ed. (Katz­man et al. 1988) They also showed high­er brain weights and greater num­ber of neu­rons when com­pared to age-matched con­trols. The inves­ti­ga­tors hypoth­e­sized that the patients had a larg­er “reserve” of neu­rons and abil­i­ties that off­set the loss­es caused by Alzheimer’s. Since then the con­cept of cog­ni­tive reserve has been defined as the abil­i­ty of an indi­vid­ual to tol­er­ate pro­gres­sive brain pathol­o­gy with­out demon­strat­ing clin­i­cal cog­ni­tive symp­toms.

Despite many stud­ies that demon­strate an asso­ci­a­tion between high­er par­tic­i­pa­tion in more intel­lec­tu­al, social and phys­i­cal activ­i­ties and more reserve, most of these stud­ies were not done over a long enough time peri­od to rule out whether the stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties were pro­mot­ing high­er cog­ni­tive per­for­mance or high­er per­form­ers were more like­ly to engage in stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties. The lon­gi­tu­di­nal stud­ies show a mutu­al­ly rein­forc­ing cycle between an ini­tial­ly high intel­lec­tu­al func­tion­ing per­son, an engaged lifestyle, and more cog­ni­tive reserve, but also a main­te­nance of intel­lec­tu­al per­for­mance. (Arbuck­le et al. 1992, Gold et al. 1995, Hultsch et al 1999, Schaie 1984, Schaie 1996, School­er and Mulatu 2001)

In one study of 1772 non­de­ment­ed indi­vid­u­als over sev­en years that con­trolled for fac­tors like eth­nic group, edu­ca­tion, and occu­pa­tion, par­tic­i­pants with high leisure activ­i­ty had 38% less risk of devel­op­ing demen­tia, and that risk was reduced by approx­i­mate­ly 12% for each addi­tion­al leisure activ­i­ty adopt­ed. (Scarmeas, Levy, et al. 2001) Lat­er stud­ies, includ­ing imag­ing stud­ies of cere­bral blood flow, con­tin­ue to build up data show­ing fre­quent par­tic­i­pa­tion in cog­ni­tive­ly stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties reduces risk for Alzheimer’s and slows the rate of cog­ni­tive decline. Inter­est­ing­ly, phys­i­cal, social, and intel­lec­tu­al activ­i­ties all help, although intel­lec­tu­al activ­i­ties were asso­ci­at­ed with the low­est risk of inci­dent demen­tia. Fur­ther­more, it has been shown that peo­ple with high cog­ni­tive reserve decline more rapid­ly, sup­port­ing the idea that the pathol­o­gy is more advanced by the time it is clin­i­cal­ly appar­ent. (Stern, Tang, et al. 1995)


The asso­ci­a­tion between engaged lifestyle and demen­tia risk could be either medi­at­ed or con­found­ed by abil­i­ties like IQ or edu­ca­tion. If this is the case then it could be that either IQ or edu­ca­tion rep­re­sent the true causal links with demen­tia or that sub­jects with high­er IQ or edu­ca­tion tend to adopt lifestyles which them­selves causal­ly reduce the risk of demen­tia (such as exer­cise, diet, etc.). Nev­er­the­less, in stud­ies where edu­ca­tion and occu­pa­tion (Scarmeas et al. 2001) or edu­ca­tion and IQ (Scarmeas et al. 2003) were con­trolled for, the asso­ci­a­tion between leisure activ­i­ties and demen­tia risk was still there.

Oth­er pos­si­bil­i­ties are that high func­tion­ing and engaged lifestyle are results of an innate capac­i­ty. Or per­haps bor­der­line demen­tia patients are less active as a result of the pathol­o­gy. Or per­haps the con­nec­tion has yet to be found.

How Does it Work?
If it is a causal rela­tion­ship, there are four pos­si­ble expla­na­tions of how it might work:

  1. Par­tic­i­pa­tion in stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties spurs neu­ronal growth and a larg­er num­ber of neu­rons to com­pen­sate for the pathol­o­gy
  2. High activ­i­ty peo­ple use the same num­ber of neur­al net­works more effi­cient­ly
  3. High activ­i­ty peo­ple use alter­nate neur­al net­works more effi­cient­ly to com­pen­sate for the pathol­o­gy
  4. The fac­tors that affect cog­ni­tive reserve dis­rupt the devel­op­ment of the dis­ease pathol­o­gy by decreas­ing neu­rode­gen­er­a­tion


Update: we now have an in-depth inter­view with Yaakov Stern, lead­ing advo­cate of the cog­ni­tive reserve the­o­ry, and one of the authors of the paper we review below: click on Build Your Cog­ni­tive Reserve-Yaakov Stern.  


Fur­ther Read­ing

Leave a Reply...

Loading Facebook Comments ...

12 Responses

  1. Allene Wright says:

    I have just been intro­duced to the the­o­ry of cog­ni­tive reserve by a psy­chi­a­trist treat­ing a friend. I need to know as much as I can about the the­o­ry and am delight­ed in this web­site.

  2. Allene, glad to hear 🙂

Leave a Reply

Categories: Cognitive Neuroscience

Tags: , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , ,

All Slidedecks & Recordings Available — click image below

Search for anything brain-related in our article archives

About SharpBrains

As seen in The New York Times, The Wall Street Journal, BBC News, CNN, Reuters, and more, SharpBrains is an independent market research firm and think tank tracking health and performance applications of brain science.