Sharp Brains: Brain Fitness and Cognitive Health News

Neuroplasticity, Brain Fitness and Cognitive Health News

Icon

Cognitive Reserve and Lifestyle

Update: we now have an in-depth inter­view with Yaakov Stern, lead­ing advo­cate of the cog­ni­tive reserve the­ory, and one of the authors of the paper we review below: click on Build Your Cog­ni­tive Reserve-Yaakov Stern. 

————————

In honor of the Week of Sci­ence pre­sented at Just Sci­ence from Mon­day, Feb­ru­ary 5, through Sun­day, Feb­ru­ary 11, we will be writ­ing about “just sci­ence” this week. We thought we would take this time to dis­cuss more deeply some of the key sci­en­tific pub­li­ca­tions in brain fitness.

Today, we will high­light the key points in an excel­lent review of cog­ni­tive reserve: Scarmeas, Niko­laos and Stern, Yaakov. Cog­ni­tive reserve and lifestyle. Jour­nal of Clin­i­cal and Exper­i­men­tal Neu­ropsy­chol­ogy. 2003;25:625–33.

What is Cog­ni­tive Reserve?
The con­cept of a cog­ni­tive reserve has been around since 1998 when a post mortem analy­sis of 137 peo­ple with Alzheimer’s Dis­ease showed that the patients exhib­ited fewer clin­i­cal symp­toms than their actual pathol­ogy sug­gested. (Katz­man et al. 1988) They also showed higher brain weights and greater num­ber of neu­rons when com­pared to age-matched con­trols. The inves­ti­ga­tors hypoth­e­sized that the patients had a larger “reserve” of neu­rons and abil­i­ties that off­set the losses caused by Alzheimer’s. Since then the con­cept of cog­ni­tive reserve has been defined as the abil­ity of an indi­vid­ual to tol­er­ate pro­gres­sive brain pathol­ogy with­out demon­strat­ing clin­i­cal cog­ni­tive symp­toms.

Despite many stud­ies that demon­strate an asso­ci­a­tion between higher par­tic­i­pa­tion in more intel­lec­tual, social and phys­i­cal activ­i­ties and more reserve, most of these stud­ies were not done over a long enough time period to rule out whether the stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties were pro­mot­ing higher cog­ni­tive per­for­mance or higher per­form­ers were more likely to engage in stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties. The lon­gi­tu­di­nal stud­ies show a mutu­ally rein­forc­ing cycle between an ini­tially high intel­lec­tual func­tion­ing per­son, an engaged lifestyle, and more cog­ni­tive reserve, but also a main­te­nance of intel­lec­tual per­for­mance. (Arbuckle et al. 1992, Gold et al. 1995, Hultsch et al 1999, Schaie 1984, Schaie 1996, Schooler and Mulatu 2001)

In one study of 1772 non­de­mented indi­vid­u­als over seven years that con­trolled for fac­tors like eth­nic group, edu­ca­tion, and occu­pa­tion, par­tic­i­pants with high leisure activ­ity had 38% less risk of devel­op­ing demen­tia, and that risk was reduced by approx­i­mately 12% for each addi­tional leisure activ­ity adopted. (Scarmeas, Levy, et al. 2001) Later stud­ies, includ­ing imag­ing stud­ies of cere­bral blood flow, con­tinue to build up data show­ing fre­quent par­tic­i­pa­tion in cog­ni­tively stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties reduces risk for Alzheimer’s and slows the rate of cog­ni­tive decline. Inter­est­ingly, phys­i­cal, social, and intel­lec­tual activ­i­ties all help, although intel­lec­tual activ­i­ties were asso­ci­ated with the low­est risk of inci­dent demen­tia. Fur­ther­more, it has been shown that peo­ple with high cog­ni­tive reserve decline more rapidly, sup­port­ing the idea that the pathol­ogy is more advanced by the time it is clin­i­cally appar­ent. (Stern, Tang, et al. 1995)

Causal­ity

The asso­ci­a­tion between engaged lifestyle and demen­tia risk could be either medi­ated or con­founded by abil­i­ties like IQ or edu­ca­tion. If this is the case then it could be that either IQ or edu­ca­tion rep­re­sent the true causal links with demen­tia or that sub­jects with higher IQ or edu­ca­tion tend to adopt lifestyles which them­selves causally reduce the risk of demen­tia (such as exer­cise, diet, etc.). Nev­er­the­less, in stud­ies where edu­ca­tion and occu­pa­tion (Scarmeas et al. 2001) or edu­ca­tion and IQ (Scarmeas et al. 2003) were con­trolled for, the asso­ci­a­tion between leisure activ­i­ties and demen­tia risk was still there.

Other pos­si­bil­i­ties are that high func­tion­ing and engaged lifestyle are results of an innate capac­ity. Or per­haps bor­der­line demen­tia patients are less active as a result of the pathol­ogy. Or per­haps the con­nec­tion has yet to be found.

How Does it Work?
If it is a causal rela­tion­ship, there are four pos­si­ble expla­na­tions of how it might work:

  1. Par­tic­i­pa­tion in stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties spurs neu­ronal growth and a larger num­ber of neu­rons to com­pen­sate for the pathology
  2. High activ­ity peo­ple use the same num­ber of neural net­works more efficiently
  3. High activ­ity peo­ple use alter­nate neural net­works more effi­ciently to com­pen­sate for the pathology
  4. The fac­tors that affect cog­ni­tive reserve dis­rupt the devel­op­ment of the dis­ease pathol­ogy by decreas­ing neurodegeneration

———————- 

Update: we now have an in-depth inter­view with Yaakov Stern, lead­ing advo­cate of the cog­ni­tive reserve the­ory, and one of the authors of the paper we review below: click on Build Your Cog­ni­tive Reserve-Yaakov Stern.  

———————-

Fur­ther Reading

Be Socia­ble, Share!
    Print This Article Print This Article

    Categories: Cognitive Neuroscience

    Tags: , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , ,

    Welcome to SharpBrains.com

    As seen in The New York Times, The Wall Street Jour­nal, CNN and more, Sharp­Brains is an inde­pen­dent mar­ket research firm track­ing health and well­ness appli­ca­tions of brain science.
    FIRST-TIME VISITOR? Dis­cover HERE the most pop­u­lar resources at SharpBrains.com

    Follow us via

    twitter_logo_header

    Gold Sponsors @ 2014 SharpBrains Virtual Summit (October 28-30th)

    Enter Your Email to receive Sharp­Brains free, monthly eNewslet­ter:
    Join more than 50,000 Sub­scribers and stay informed and engaged.